People have argued for centuries about the relationship between thought and language. Does our language limit or change the way we think? Do people with very different languages see life differently, as well?
For most of history, people could not prove their arguments about how language affects thinking. Linguists have recently developed tools and made discoveries that provide some answers.
This TED talk is an excellent introduction to the question.
Ms. Boroditsky discusses an aboriginal Australian culture which has no words for left or right. Instead, any explanation of location uses the cardinal directions (east, west, north, or south.)
Even to say ‘hello’ you need to give the direction in which you are going.
As a result, everyone who speaks that language will stay oriented in space in a way most of us do not.
She also discusses language differences in counting (or not counting), color perception, gender assigned to nouns, and the ways we talk about accidents. These differences have very definite consequences.
Experiments have shown that people whose language emphasizes that an action was an accident remember that. English speakers are more likely to remember who did it, “because English requires you to say, ‘He did it; he broke the vase.’"
In a second article, a multi-lingual woman discusses the differences between various languages. (For example, she suggests that English encourages informality since it has only one word for ‘you’ whether talking to a close friend or a supervisor.)
She mentions favorite words in each language. ((Her exchange host mother in Ecuador teased her about getting up late. The mother claimed her difficulty is because her previous languages lacked the verb madrugar (to get up very early).
Her conclusion: “This year has only proved to me that languages we speak shape the way we act and express ourselves.”
In Steven Pinker’s TED talk, he explains several ways that language can express—or sometimes cover up—our thoughts.
He starts with the linguistic difference between "Give a muffin to a mouse," and "give a mouse a muffin." (I suspect he chose this example because of a famous children’s picture book with the second name. Most people don't give muffins to mice or to other pests (unwanted animals.)
He also explores why people may use intentionally ambiguous language. It can protect them when they are not sure how the other person will react. (The ambiguity is deliberate. Because their message was unclear, they can deny part of it if it provokes anger or threats.)
Pinker gives as an example the communication during a traffic stop in the movie Fargo.
A kidnapper is stopped for a traffic violation by a police officer. He would like to bribe the officer (give him money illegally) so he will leave quickly and not discover the bigger crime. However, the kidnapper will be in more trouble if he offers a bribe and the officer is honest.
So, when he gets out his wallet to show his driver’s license, he holds it so that a 50-dollar bill shows under it. A dishonest officer will take the hint, accept the money, and leave. An honest officer will not take the money, but he cannot prove that the man was trying to bribe him.
Since the kidnapper doesn’t know if the officer is honest or not, he limits his risk by keeping his message ambiguous.
In the last 40 years, most linguists have argued that languages are different but not superior or inferior to each other. Even “creole” language blends have definite rules and are not just an ignorant version of a “good” language.
In this explanation of Singlish, the author argues that it’s a useful language.(Singlish is Singaporean English, with some elements of Chinese and Malay.) He understands why the government of Singapore wants its people to be able to use “standard” English. That will make it easier for them to do business with people in the rest of the world.
However, he feels the government needs to stop campaigning against Singlish. He argues that, like African-American Vernacular English, Singlish has been considered a second-class language. He believes, as linguists have insisted, that both should get credit for the creative, fully-formed languages they are.
This article on gender-neutral language points out the changing use of 'they' in English. Many English speakers have begun using ‘they’ as a singular when talking about an unknown person. Using ‘they’ for one person is awkward, but so is using ‘he’ when a new boss or sales rep might be a woman...
We also use ‘Ms.’ more and more when we’re not sure if the woman we are speaking to is married or not, or if she might object to ‘Miss’ or ‘Mrs.”
The author believes that such changes may slowly change our thinking and reduce male dominance. He bases that conclusion on the results of a Swedish experiment with a gender-neutral word.
You can practice some of the vocabulary from these articles with a crossword on Language and Linguistics. The answers are here.
Here are a few explanations of other words that you might not recognize. (Most of these are not on the crossword-- or at least not fully explained there):
The Singlish article offers a good example of a context clue for the meaning of creole. “Singlish can broadly be categorized as a creole, which is a full language that arises suddenly, usually with one language as its base, but with unique grammatical features and many words from at least one other language.” (Paragraph 3. It goes on to explain the difference between pidgin and creole.)
After watching this 5 1/2-minute video, answer a few comprehension questions, then “dig deeper” with some excellent ideas and additional resources.
Home > Comprehension Exercises > The Relationship between Thought and Language
Didn't find what you
needed? Explain what you want in the search box below.
(For example, cognates, past tense practice, or 'get along with.') Click to see the related pages on EnglishHints.
site search by freefind | advanced |
New! Comments
What do you think about what you just read? Leave me a comment in the box below.